Hermann Ackermann, Wolfram Ziegler
Audra Ames, Sara Wielandt, Dianne Cameron, Stan Kuczaj
David Ardell, Noelle Anderson, Bodo Winter
Rie Asano, Edward Ruoyang Shi
Mark Atkinson, Kenny Smith, Simon Kirby
Andreas Baumann, Christina Prömer, Kamil Kazmierski, Nikolaus Ritt
Christian Bentz
Aleksandrs Berdicevskis, Hanne Eckhoff
Richard A. Blythe, Alistair H. Jones, Jessica Renton
Cedric Boeckx, Constantina Theofanopoulou, Antonio Benítez-Burraco
Megan Broadway, Jamie Klaus, Billie Serafin, Heidi Lyn
Jon W. Carr, Kenny Smith, Hannah Cornish, Simon Kirby
Federica Cavicchio, Livnat Leemor, Simone Shamay-Tsoory, Wendy Sandler
Zanna Clay, Jahmaira Archbold, Klaus Zuberbuhler
Katie Collier, Andrew N. Radford, Balthasar Bickel, Marta B. Manser, Simon W. Townsend
Jennifer Culbertson, Simon Kirby, Marieke Schouwstra
Christine Cuskley, Vittorio Loreto
Christine Cuskley, Bernardo Monechi, Pietro Gravino, Vittorio Loreto
Dan Dediu, Scott Moisik
Sabrina Engesser, Amanda R. Ridley, Simon W. Townsend
Dankmar Enke, Roland Mühlenbernd, Igor Yanovich
Kerem Eryilmaz, Hannah Little, Bart de Boer
Nicolas Fay, Shane Rogers
Maryia Fedzechkina, Becky Chu, T. Florian Jaeger, John Trueswell
Olga Feher, Kenny Smith, Elizabeth Wonnacott, Nikolaus Ritt
Piera Filippi, Sebastian Ocklenburg, Daniel Liu Bowling, Larissa Heege, Albert Newen, Onur Güntürkün, Bart de Boer
Piera Filippi, Jenna V. Congdon, John Hoang, Daniel Liu Bowling, Stephan Reber, Andrius Pašukonis, Marisa Hoeschele, Sebastian Ocklenburg, Bart de Boer, Christopher B. Sturdy, Albert Newen, Onur GÜntÜrkÜn
Molly Flaherty, Katelyn Stangl, Susan Goldin-Meadow
Marlen Fröhlich, Paul H Kuchenbuch, Gudrun Müller, Barbara Fruth, Takeshi Furuichi, Roman M Wittig, Simone Pika
Victor Gay, Daniel Hicks, Estefania Santacreu-Vasut
Andreea Geambasu, Michelle J. Spierings, Carel ten Cate, Clara C. Levelt
Matt Hall, Russell Richie, Marie Coppola
Stefan Hartmann, Peeter Tinits, Jonas Nölle, Thomas Hartmann, Michael Pleyer
Wolfram Hinzen, Joana Rosselló
Rick Janssen, Bodo Winter, Dan Dediu, Scott Moisik, Sean Roberts
Rick Janssen, Dan Dediu, Scott Moisik
Jasmeen Kanwal, Kenny Smith, Jennifer Culbertson, Simon Kirby
Deborah Kerr, Kenny Smith
Buddhamas Kriengwatana, Paola Escudero, Anne Kerkhoven, Carel ten Cate
Adriano Lameira, Jeremy Kendal, Marco Gamba
Molly Lewis, Michael C. Frank
Casey Lister, Tiarn Burtenshaw, Nicolas Fay, Bradley Walker, Jeneva Ohan
Hannah Little, Kerem Eryılmaz, Bart de Boer
Hannah Little, Kerem Eryılmaz, Bart de Boer
Giuseppe Longobardi, Armin Buch, Andrea Ceolin, Aaron Ecay, Cristina Guardiano, Monica Irimia, Dimitris Michelioudakis, Nina Radkevich, Gerhard Jaeger
Heidi Lyn, Stephanie Jett, Megan Broadway, Mystera Samuelson
Michael Mcloughlin, Luca Lamoni, Ellen Garland, Simon Ingram, Alexis Kirke, Michael Noad, Luke Rendell, Eduardo Miranda
Adrien Meguerditchian, Damien Marie, Konstantina Margiotoudi, Scott A. Love, Alice Bertello, Romain Lacoste, Muriel Roth, Bruno Nazarian, Jean-Luc Anton, Olivier Coulon
Jérôme Michaud
Ashley Micklos
Marie Montant, Johannes Ziegler, Benny Briesemeister, Tila Brink, Bruno Wicker, Aurélie Ponz, Mireille Bonnard, Arthur Jacobs, Mario Braun
Yasamin Motamedi, Marieke Schouwstra, Kenny Smith, Simon Kirby
Roland Mühlenbernd, Johannes Wahle
Tomoya Nakai, Kazuo Okanoya
Savithry Namboodiripad, Daniel Lenzen, Ryan Lepic, Tessa Verhoef
Alan Nielsen, Dieuwke Hupkes, Simon Kirby, Kenny Smith
Bill Noble, Raquel Fernández
Irene M. Pepperberg, Katia Zilber-Izhar, Scott Smith
Lynn Perry, Marcus Perlman, Gary Lupyan, Bodo Winter, Dominic Massaro
Ljiljana Progovac
Andrea Ravignani, Tania Delgado, Simon Kirby
Terry Regier, Alexandra Carstensen, Charles Kemp
Lilia Rissman, Laura Horton, Molly Flaherty, Marie Coppola, Annie Senghas, Diane Brentari, Susan Goldin-Meadow
Gareth Roberts, Mariya Fedzechkina
Carmen Saldana, Simon Kirby, Kenny Smith
Carlos Santana
William Schueller, Pierre-Yves Oudeyer
Catriona Silvey, Christos Christodoulopoulos
Katie Slocombe, Stuart Watson, Anne Schel, Claudia Wilke, Emma Wallace, Leveda Cheng, Victoria West, Simon Townsend
Ruth Sonnweber, Andrea Ravignani
Michelle Spierings, Carel ten Cate
Kevin Stadler, Elyse Jamieson, Kenny Smith, Simon Kirby
Monica Tamariz, Joleana Shurley
Monica Tamariz, Jon W. Carr
Bill Thompson, Heikki Rasilo
Oksana Tkachman, Carla L. Hudson Kam
Simon Townsend, Andrew Russell, Sabrina Engesser
Francesca Tria, Vittorio Loreto, Vito Servedio, S. Mufwene Salikoko
Anu Vastenius, Jordan Zlatev, Joost Van de Weijer
Tessa Verhoef, Carol Padden, Simon Kirby
Slawomir Wacewicz, Przemyslaw Zywiczynski, Arkadiusz Jasinski
Bodo Winter, David Ardell
Bodo Winter, Lynn Perry, Marcus Perlman, Gary Lupyan
Marieke Woensdregt, Kenny Smith, Chris Cummins, Simon Kirby
Eva Zehentner, Andreas Baumann, Nikolaus Ritt, Christina Prömer
Keywords: Ostension, Primate, Gesture
Abstract:
The ability to mark a particular behavior as a communicative act, rather than relying on a small set of phylogenetically-shaped signals, exponentially expands the potential of a communicative system. Essentially any behavior could be made communicative through ostension. When paired with the ability to infer meaning from novel contexts and behaviors, this generates a powerful communicative engine. Human language is arguably built upon just such an ostensive-inferential engine (Sperber & Wilson, 1995; Origgi and Sperber 2000; Scott-Phillips, 2014). The ability to take an action or sound and imbue it with meaning through “performing” it as a signal is undoubtedly an integral part of modern human language. But is it uniquely human?
Recently, Scott-Phillips extended the discussion of the O-I system by systematically contrasting the communication systems of great apes and humans in regards to their properties as codes (Scott-Phillips, 2014, 2015). He argues that ape communication is a “natural code,” relying on associative mechanisms and expanded by metapsychological abilities. Human language is a “conventional code,” built upon metapsychological abilities (the O-I system) and made more powerful by associative mechanisms. This contrast between natural and conventional codes makes important predictions about the communicative behavior of apes and humans, particularly that only humans possess and recognize communicative intentions (an act that provides information that it is communicative – i.e., signaling its own signalhood). This capacity, in turn, lies at the heart of the O-I system. However, the evidence that ape communication is a natural code (and not based on communicative intentions) comes from the published literature on ape communication. This is problematic, because the code model itself has had a dramatic impact on the work that is conducted (and published) in primate communication.
Primate communication studies search for and highlight predictable forms and contingencies that might be interpretable as codes. Ape gestural communication is less predictable and more flexible than the communication systems of many other animals, but while presence of communicative flexibility is used as evidence of intentionality in ape gesture literature (Call & Tomasello, 2007), the ambiguities themselves are often discarded or overlooked. Apes use their gestures flexibly, modifying them in response to their communicative partners: they direct their signals towards others, account for their partner’s gaze, and wait for a response after gesturing, and yet the majority of published papers focus on predictability of signal to response (i.e. searching for codes).
I will review common data analysis procedures in ape gesture research (e.g., Cartmill & Byrne, 2010) and discuss the ways in which they influence the perception that gestures are natural communicative codes. I will present gestures that are typically deemed “unanalyzable.” Many of these are rare or ambiguous gestures that do not show a simple one form to one meaning mapping. These examples have the greatest potential to demonstrate ostensive communication in great apes. The theory that human communication is built on a framework of ostension and inference is compelling, but to determine whether humans are unique in these abilities we must assess the lasting impact of the code model framework on studies of primate communication. Primatologists should tackle this challenge head on. Emerging meta-analytic tools may facilitate this analysis by pooling rare events across studies and detecting complex regularities. These approaches would make significant advances in our understanding of the relationship between primate communication and human language.
References
Call J, Tomasello M (2007) The gestural communication of apes and monkeys. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc, Mahwah.
Cartmill, E., & Byrne, R. (2010). Semantics of primate gestures: intentional meanings of orangutan gestures. Animal cognition, 13(6), 793-804.
Origgi, G., and D. Sperber. (2000). Evolution, communication and the proper function of language. In Evolution and the human mind: language, modu- larity and social cognition. P. Carruthers and A. Chamberlain, eds. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Pp. 140–169.
Scott-Phillips, T. (2014). Speaking Our Minds: Why human communication is different, and how language evolved to make it special. Palgrave MacMillan.
Scott-Phillips, T. (2015). Nonhuman primate communication, pragmatics, and the origins of language. Current Anthropology, 56(1), 56-80.
Sperber, D., and D. Wilson. (1995). Relevance: communication and cognition. 2nd edition. Oxford: Blackwell.
Citation:
Cartmill E. (2016). Lasting Impacts Of The Code Model On Primate Communication Research. In S.G. Roberts, C. Cuskley, L. McCrohon, L. Barceló-Coblijn, O. Fehér & T. Verhoef (eds.) The Evolution of Language: Proceedings of the 11th International Conference (EVOLANG11). Available online: http://evolang.org/neworleans/papers/170.html