Hermann Ackermann, Wolfram Ziegler
Audra Ames, Sara Wielandt, Dianne Cameron, Stan Kuczaj
David Ardell, Noelle Anderson, Bodo Winter
Rie Asano, Edward Ruoyang Shi
Mark Atkinson, Kenny Smith, Simon Kirby
Andreas Baumann, Christina Prömer, Kamil Kazmierski, Nikolaus Ritt
Christian Bentz
Aleksandrs Berdicevskis, Hanne Eckhoff
Richard A. Blythe, Alistair H. Jones, Jessica Renton
Cedric Boeckx, Constantina Theofanopoulou, Antonio Benítez-Burraco
Megan Broadway, Jamie Klaus, Billie Serafin, Heidi Lyn
Jon W. Carr, Kenny Smith, Hannah Cornish, Simon Kirby
Federica Cavicchio, Livnat Leemor, Simone Shamay-Tsoory, Wendy Sandler
Zanna Clay, Jahmaira Archbold, Klaus Zuberbuhler
Katie Collier, Andrew N. Radford, Balthasar Bickel, Marta B. Manser, Simon W. Townsend
Jennifer Culbertson, Simon Kirby, Marieke Schouwstra
Christine Cuskley, Vittorio Loreto
Christine Cuskley, Bernardo Monechi, Pietro Gravino, Vittorio Loreto
Dan Dediu, Scott Moisik
Sabrina Engesser, Amanda R. Ridley, Simon W. Townsend
Dankmar Enke, Roland Mühlenbernd, Igor Yanovich
Kerem Eryilmaz, Hannah Little, Bart de Boer
Nicolas Fay, Shane Rogers
Maryia Fedzechkina, Becky Chu, T. Florian Jaeger, John Trueswell
Olga Feher, Kenny Smith, Elizabeth Wonnacott, Nikolaus Ritt
Piera Filippi, Sebastian Ocklenburg, Daniel Liu Bowling, Larissa Heege, Albert Newen, Onur Güntürkün, Bart de Boer
Piera Filippi, Jenna V. Congdon, John Hoang, Daniel Liu Bowling, Stephan Reber, Andrius Pašukonis, Marisa Hoeschele, Sebastian Ocklenburg, Bart de Boer, Christopher B. Sturdy, Albert Newen, Onur GÜntÜrkÜn
Molly Flaherty, Katelyn Stangl, Susan Goldin-Meadow
Marlen Fröhlich, Paul H Kuchenbuch, Gudrun Müller, Barbara Fruth, Takeshi Furuichi, Roman M Wittig, Simone Pika
Victor Gay, Daniel Hicks, Estefania Santacreu-Vasut
Andreea Geambasu, Michelle J. Spierings, Carel ten Cate, Clara C. Levelt
Matt Hall, Russell Richie, Marie Coppola
Stefan Hartmann, Peeter Tinits, Jonas Nölle, Thomas Hartmann, Michael Pleyer
Wolfram Hinzen, Joana Rosselló
Rick Janssen, Bodo Winter, Dan Dediu, Scott Moisik, Sean Roberts
Rick Janssen, Dan Dediu, Scott Moisik
Jasmeen Kanwal, Kenny Smith, Jennifer Culbertson, Simon Kirby
Deborah Kerr, Kenny Smith
Buddhamas Kriengwatana, Paola Escudero, Anne Kerkhoven, Carel ten Cate
Adriano Lameira, Jeremy Kendal, Marco Gamba
Molly Lewis, Michael C. Frank
Casey Lister, Tiarn Burtenshaw, Nicolas Fay, Bradley Walker, Jeneva Ohan
Hannah Little, Kerem Eryılmaz, Bart de Boer
Hannah Little, Kerem Eryılmaz, Bart de Boer
Giuseppe Longobardi, Armin Buch, Andrea Ceolin, Aaron Ecay, Cristina Guardiano, Monica Irimia, Dimitris Michelioudakis, Nina Radkevich, Gerhard Jaeger
Heidi Lyn, Stephanie Jett, Megan Broadway, Mystera Samuelson
Michael Mcloughlin, Luca Lamoni, Ellen Garland, Simon Ingram, Alexis Kirke, Michael Noad, Luke Rendell, Eduardo Miranda
Adrien Meguerditchian, Damien Marie, Konstantina Margiotoudi, Scott A. Love, Alice Bertello, Romain Lacoste, Muriel Roth, Bruno Nazarian, Jean-Luc Anton, Olivier Coulon
Jérôme Michaud
Ashley Micklos
Marie Montant, Johannes Ziegler, Benny Briesemeister, Tila Brink, Bruno Wicker, Aurélie Ponz, Mireille Bonnard, Arthur Jacobs, Mario Braun
Yasamin Motamedi, Marieke Schouwstra, Kenny Smith, Simon Kirby
Roland Mühlenbernd, Johannes Wahle
Tomoya Nakai, Kazuo Okanoya
Savithry Namboodiripad, Daniel Lenzen, Ryan Lepic, Tessa Verhoef
Alan Nielsen, Dieuwke Hupkes, Simon Kirby, Kenny Smith
Bill Noble, Raquel Fernández
Irene M. Pepperberg, Katia Zilber-Izhar, Scott Smith
Lynn Perry, Marcus Perlman, Gary Lupyan, Bodo Winter, Dominic Massaro
Ljiljana Progovac
Andrea Ravignani, Tania Delgado, Simon Kirby
Terry Regier, Alexandra Carstensen, Charles Kemp
Lilia Rissman, Laura Horton, Molly Flaherty, Marie Coppola, Annie Senghas, Diane Brentari, Susan Goldin-Meadow
Gareth Roberts, Mariya Fedzechkina
Carmen Saldana, Simon Kirby, Kenny Smith
Carlos Santana
William Schueller, Pierre-Yves Oudeyer
Catriona Silvey, Christos Christodoulopoulos
Katie Slocombe, Stuart Watson, Anne Schel, Claudia Wilke, Emma Wallace, Leveda Cheng, Victoria West, Simon Townsend
Ruth Sonnweber, Andrea Ravignani
Michelle Spierings, Carel ten Cate
Kevin Stadler, Elyse Jamieson, Kenny Smith, Simon Kirby
Monica Tamariz, Joleana Shurley
Monica Tamariz, Jon W. Carr
Bill Thompson, Heikki Rasilo
Oksana Tkachman, Carla L. Hudson Kam
Simon Townsend, Andrew Russell, Sabrina Engesser
Francesca Tria, Vittorio Loreto, Vito Servedio, S. Mufwene Salikoko
Anu Vastenius, Jordan Zlatev, Joost Van de Weijer
Tessa Verhoef, Carol Padden, Simon Kirby
Slawomir Wacewicz, Przemyslaw Zywiczynski, Arkadiusz Jasinski
Bodo Winter, David Ardell
Bodo Winter, Lynn Perry, Marcus Perlman, Gary Lupyan
Marieke Woensdregt, Kenny Smith, Chris Cummins, Simon Kirby
Eva Zehentner, Andreas Baumann, Nikolaus Ritt, Christina Prömer
Keywords: Constituency, Hierarchical structure, Dendrophilia hypothesis, Bonobo
Abstract:
Fitch (2014) proposed the Dendrophilia hypothesis as a description of the ubiquity of hierarchical structures in human cognition:
‘Humans have a multi-domain capacity and proclivity to infer tree structures from strings, to a degree that is difficult or impossible for most non-human animal species.’ (Fitch, 2014, 352)
Part of Fitch’s supporting evidence concerns Fitch and Hauser’s (2004) demonstration that humans learn to recognize sequences of the forms (ab)^n and a^nb^n, while cotton-top tamarins can only learn the former. Fitch takes this to support the Dendrophilia hypothesis because (ab)^n, but not a^nb^n, can be generated in the limit by constituency-free finite-state grammars. However, this result has been disputed on empirical and theoretical grounds, e.g. Perruchet and Rey (2005), Jäger and Rogers (2012).
This paper gives a complementary source of evidence for Fitch’s hypothesis. We examine a corpus of 660 utterances directed in parallel to a bonobo, Kanzi, and a human infant, Alia, together with descriptions of their behavior in response to those utterances (Savage-Rumbaugh et al., 1993). Unlike grammar induction experiments such as Fitch and Hauser (2004), these strings are paired with interpretations. We can then infer aspects of a subject’s interpretation of an utterance from their behavior, and aspects of the grammatical representation of the utterance from that interpretation. I argue that Kanzi fails to respond to requests precisely where correct interpretation requires hierarchical constituency.
Kanzi’s overall performance across the corpus (71.5% ‘correct’ responses according to Savage-Rumbaugh et al.’s criteria) is comparable to Alia’s (66.6% ‘correct’). Usually, though, a correct response could be achieved through common-sense combination of the concepts expressed by individual words, without using syntactic information (Anderson’s 2004 semantic soup strategy). One such example, carried out correctly by Kanzi, is "Put the backpack in the car": few other actions involving backpacks and cars suggest themselves.
In some cases (e.g. "Put the tomato in the oil" / "Put some oil in the tomato"), correct interpretation requires sensitivity to linear order, but not constituency. Kanzi’s accuracy on 43 such sentences in the corpus (21 pairs, with 1 example repeated) is 76.7%, in line with his 71.5% overall accuracy. This suggests that Kanzi can make use of linear order information in his understanding of spoken English.
However, Kanzi responded correctly to only 4/18 sentences containing coordinated NP objects (22.2%). When asked to "Show me the milk and the doggie", he shows only the dog; when asked to "Give the lighter and the shoe to Rose", he gives Rose only the lighter. Kanzi ignores the first conjunct on 9/18 trials, and ignores the second conjunct on 5/18 trials.
Despite the small number of critical sentences, this represents a highly significant drop relative to both Kanzi’s baseline accuracy (p < 10^−4) and Alia’s 68.4% accuracy on sentences containing the same construction (p < 0.01). This, then, is a species-specific, construction-specific drop in performance.
I propose that Kanzi’s performance dips precisely here (and not on many other constructions of comparable length) because correct interpretation of such sentences requires reference to hierarchical constituent structure. Specifically, unlike the previous examples, Kanzi must recognize that the object of give is the complex phrase the water and the doggie, and not just, for example, the next noun. Likewise, the patient of the action of giving should be the group of objects denoted by the complex phrase, not just the denotaton of a single noun. Kanzi’s generally impressive performance therefore only drops where reference to constituency is required, while Alia has no similar problem. In Fitch’s terms, Kanzi is more dendrophobic than Alia.
References
Anderson, S. (2004). Doctor Doolittle’s delusion. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Fitch, W. T. (2014). Toward a computational framework for cognitive biology: Unifying approaches from cognitive neuoscience and comparative cognition. Physics of Life Reviews, 11, 329-364.
Fitch, W. T., & Hauser, M. (2004). Computational constraints on syntactic processing in a nonhuman primate. Science, 303, 377-380.
Jäger, G., & Rogers, J. (2012). Formal language theory: Refining the Chomsky hierarchy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 367, 1956- 1970.
Perruchet, P., & Rey, A. (2005). Does the mastery of center-embedded linguistic structures distinguish humans from nonhuman primates? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 12, 307-313.
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Murphy, J., Sevcik, R., Brakke, K., Williams, S., Rumbaugh, D., & Bates, E. (1993). Language comprehension in ape and child. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, 1-252.
Citation:
Truswell R. (2016). Dendrophobia In Bonobo Comprehension Of Spoken English. In S.G. Roberts, C. Cuskley, L. McCrohon, L. Barceló-Coblijn, O. Fehér & T. Verhoef (eds.) The Evolution of Language: Proceedings of the 11th International Conference (EVOLANG11). Available online: http://evolang.org/neworleans/papers/87.html